Disrupted Timing of Discontinuous But Not

contrast, discontinuous movements require an explicit representation of the temporal goal, a .... variable than controls on the discrete tasks, yet there was no ...
3MB taille 2 téléchargements 390 vues
REPORTS

Disrupted Timing of Discontinuous But Not Continuous Movements by Cerebellar Lesions Rebecca M. C. Spencer,1,2* Howard N. Zelaznik,2 Jo¨rn Diedrichsen,1 Richard B. Ivry1 Patients with cerebellar damage are known to exhibit deficits in the temporal control of movements. We report that these deficits are restricted to discontinuous movements. Cerebellar patients exhibited no deficit in temporal variability when producing continuous, rhythmic movements. We hypothesize that the temporal properties of continuous movements are emergent and reflect the operation of other control parameters not associated with the cerebellum. In contrast, discontinuous movements require an explicit representation of the temporal goal, a function of the cerebellum. The requirement for explicit temporal representation provides a parsimonious account of cerebellar involvement in a range of tasks. Lesions of the cerebellum disrupt the temporal properties of voluntary movements. Individuals with cerebellar lesions exhibit increased temporal variability on numerous motor tasks (1–3) and are impaired on nonmotor tasks that require a precise temporal representation (4–9). Neuroimaging has provided further evidence of a cerebellar role in motor and nonmotor timing (10– 12). These results support the hypothesis that the cerebellum is part of an internal timing system in the millisecond-to-second range (13), which provides a representation of the interval to be timed. Although there is substantial evidence for the role of the cerebellum in temporal processing across domains, it has been assumed that the temporal properties of many movements may be emergent, reflecting the operation of nontemporal control parameters (14–16). Evidence of such a distinction comes from studies in which participants made repetitive tapping or circledrawing movements (17). Contrary to the prediction of a single timing process, temporal variability did not correlate across these tasks (18, 19). However, when participants were required to pause between each cycle while circling, a manipulation designed to mimic pauses in tapping, temporal variability significantly correlated with variability in tapping (20). Importantly, intermittent circle drawing did not correlate with continuous circle drawing. We hypothesized that the temporal control of continuous movements is emergent, whereas discontinuous movements require an explicit representation of the temporal goal, a 1 Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, 3210 Tolman Hall #1650, Berkeley, CA 94720, USA. 2Department of Health & Kinesiology, Integrative Program in Neuroscience, Purdue University, 1362 Lambert, West Lafayette, IN 47907, USA.

*To whom correspondence should be addressed. Email: [email protected]

function provided by the cerebellum (21). To test this hypothesis, we examined the performance of patients with cerebellar lesions on these two classes of movements. In the first experiment, six patients with unilateral cerebellar lesions (fig. S1 and table S1) were tested on repetitive movement tasks with a target cycle duration of 800 ms (22). One task was finger tapping on a tabletop; the other two tasks involved circle drawing. For continuous circle drawing, participants were instructed to draw circles in a continuous, smooth manner; for intermittent circle drawing, participants were to pause before drawing each circle. Each task was performed with the right and left limbs in separate blocks, allowing for a within-subject comparison of performance with the impaired, ipsilesional limb to performance with the less impaired, contralesional limb. Like healthy adults (18–20), the cerebellar patients paused briefly before the downstroke

of the index finger in the tapping task (Fig. 1A). The duration of this pause is not as long as that observed during intermittent circle drawing; nonetheless, its presence verifies that performance in these two conditions consisted of a series of discontinuous movements. The mean interval produced in the table-tapping task was close to the goal of 800 ms (table S2). For the circle-drawing tasks, the means were considerably longer, similar to durations produced by healthy adults (20). Because temporal variability increases with cycle duration (23), we used the coefficient of variation, defined as the standard deviation of the cycle durations divided by the mean cycle duration, as a normalized measure of temporal variability (24). Variability was significantly greater for the ipsilesional limb on the discontinuous tasks (Fig. 1B). In contrast, no difference was observed between limbs on the continuous drawing task (25). The coefficient of variation for continuous circling for both limbs was 4 to 5%, similar to that observed in healthy young adults (18–20). To verify that the patients’ deficit was related to movement discontinuity, we modified the discontinuous tasks in a second experiment in which each action was produced discretely, in an attempt to produce one tap interval or one circle cycle in 550 ms. The continuous drawing task was identical to that of Experiment 1, except that the target interval was 550 ms (table S3). Patients with unilateral cerebellar damage again exhibited a selective increase in temporal variability on the discrete tasks when performing with the impaired limb relative to the unimpaired limb (Fig. 2). There was no between-limb difference on the continuous drawing task. A similar pattern was observed in a comparison between patients with bilateral cerebellar degeneration (n ⫽ 7) and healthy control par-

Fig. 1. Disrupted timing of discontinuous but not continuous movements by cerebellar lesions. (A) Representative kinematic trajectories produced by a cerebellar patient in Experiment 1. The onset of each cycle is marked by the gray vertical lines. Top row: Cycle duration for tapping was defined by the interval between successive contact points with the table. Middle row: For intermittent circling, the onset of the movement phase was marked when velocity exceeded 5 cm/s; the onset of the pause was marked when velocity fell below 5 cm/s (dashed vertical lines). Bottom row: For continuous circling, cycle onset was defined as the point of maximum displacement in the y dimension. (B) Coefficient of variation scores for the tapping (TT), intermittent circle drawing (IC), and continuous circle drawing (CC) tasks. The patients were more variable ( paired t tests) when performing with their ipsilesional, impaired hand on tapping (P ⫽ 0.02) and intermittent drawing (P ⬍ 0.001) tasks. No impairment was evident on the continuous circle drawing task (P ⬎ 0.4). Error bars refer to the standard error of the mean, a measure of between-subject variability.

www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 300 30 MAY 2003

1437

REPORTS ticipants (n ⫽ 7). These patients were more variable than controls on the discrete tasks, yet there was no significant difference in performance between these groups on the continuous drawing task. The lack of impairment in the patients’ performance on the continuous drawing task in these two experiments is striking, given that these movements involve multiple joints, whereas finger tapping involves a single joint. As noted in the classic writings of Holmes (26), the effects of cerebellar lesions are most evident in complex movements. If difficulty were based on the number of joints and resulting interactional torques (27), or on the spatial demands of the tasks, tapping should be easier than continuous drawing. However, temporal performance on tapping was impaired, whereas continuous drawing was unimpaired. Given that ballistic movements are most affected by cerebellar lesions (28) and that patients had difficulty in matching the target intervals in Experiment 2, we designed a

third experiment to assess whether the variable that mediated performance was velocity rather than the distinction between continuous and discontinuous movements. All of the tasks were restricted to flexion/extension movements of the index finger. One condition required table tapping similar to Experiment 1. For the other conditions, the participants were instructed to “tap” in the air, moving the index finger in either a smooth, continuous manner or by pausing briefly before each downstroke (Fig. 3A). A 1000-ms target duration was used for all movements (table tapping, intermittent movement, and continuous movement). In addition, the continuous task was performed with a 500-ms target duration. We used the synchronizationcontinuation procedure from Experiment 1; for conditions other than table tapping, participants were instructed to synchronize flexion with each tone (22). We tested patients with unilateral cerebellar lesions (n ⫽ 5) or bilateral cerebellar degeneration (n ⫽ 5) and control participants (n ⫽ 5). Fig. 2. Patients with cerebellar lesions exhibited increased temporal variability only on tasks that involved discontinuous movements, discrete tapping (DT), or discrete circle drawing (DC). Unilateral cerebellar patients, between-limbs variability in DT: P ⫽ 0.01; in DC: P ⬍ 0.001. Bilateral cerebellar patients compared to the controls (unpaired t tests), in DT: P ⫽ 0.001; in DC: P ⬍ 0.001. The bilateral patients and controls performed the task with both limbs; no effect of hand was obtained for either group, so the data were averaged across limbs.

Fig. 3. Normal timing on continuous tasks held across a range of movement velocities. (A) Kinematic trajectories from one control participant on the 1000-ms intermittent and the 500-ms continuous movement tasks. The movement phase was similar for the two conditions. Cycle onset for the “air-tapping” conditions was determined as the point when movement velocity exceeded 5 cm/s (vertical lines). (B) Maximum velocity was calculated for each downstroke and averaged across cycles. IM, intermittent movement; CS, slow continuous movement (1000-ms target); CF, fast continuous movement (500-ms target). (C) Coefficient of variation scores. Within-subject comparisons (paired t tests) for the unilateral patients were only significant on the discontinuous tasks (T T: P ⫽ 0.02; IM: P ⫽ 0.008). A similar pattern was obtained in the between-subject comparison (unpaired t tests) of bilateral patients and controls (T T: P ⫽ 0.04; IM: P ⫽ 0.005).

1438

Mean durations approximated the target for all conditions (table S4). The maximum velocity increased by at least 67% in the fast (500 ms) continuous condition compared to the slow (1000 ms) continuous condition (Fig. 3B). Moreover, maximum velocity was similar for the intermittent and fast continuous conditions. Nonetheless, patients with unilateral cerebellar damage exhibited a temporal impairment on the intermittent and table-tapping tasks but were unimpaired on the continuous tasks (Fig. 3C). Providing additional evidence against a velocity-based account, the unilateral patients were more consistent when moving at the faster rate on the continuous task and there was no interaction between the response hand and velocity. Results for the patients with bilateral degeneration also favor movement continuity as the critical variable. Although the group means for bilateral cerebellar patients indicate greater variability on all tasks relative to controls, statistically this group was only more variable than the controls on the discontinuous tasks. These patients also failed to exhibit an increased impairment in the fast continuous task compared to the slow continuous task. These results provide neuropsychological evidence of dissociable neural systems for the temporal control of discontinuous and continuous movements, with the cerebellum selectively involved in the former. Converging evidence for the distinct neural control of discontinuous and continuous movements is provided by a study of bimanual coordination after callosotomy (29). These patients’ hands were temporally coupled on a bimanual version of the intermittent tapping task in Experiment 3. In contrast, the two hands became uncoupled when the movements were continuous. Discontinuous movements are characterized by salient events such as onsets and offsets or contact with the table while tapping. We hypothesize that temporal control of these tasks is achieved by the setting of explicit temporal goals for successive events and that the cerebellum is essential for this representation. In contrast, the goal for the continuous task is to move smoothly while achieving the directed pace. We hypothesize that for these movements, the temporal properties are emergent (30). The cerebellum may be involved in the initial establishment of the temporal goal, but once initiated, performance is controlled by other parameters that satisfy constraints such as minimizing jerk (31) or spatial noise (32), through a control parameter such as joint stiffness (33). In this manner, what was an explicit goal becomes an emergent property. Conceptualizing cerebellar function as representing the timing of salient events provides a parsimonious account of the involvement of this structure in both motor and

30 MAY 2003 VOL 300 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org

REPORTS nonmotor tasks. Eyeblink conditioning requires that an animal not only learn the association between a neutral and an aversive stimulus; it is only adaptive if the animal also learns when to expect the aversive stimulus. More generally, the consolidation of motor skills centers on learning to specify the precise timing between successive movements. Cerebellar ataxia is characterized as a disruption in the timing of these events (3, 26, 28), rather than as a loss of the conceptual knowledge for actions observed in apraxia. Similarly, comparing the duration of two successive events requires a judgment of whether the second event occurred earlier or later than expected. Our results extend previous theories concerning the role of the cerebellum in temporal processing, indicating that this function is limited to tasks that require an explicit specification of the timing of behaviorally meaningful events. References and Notes

1. R. B. Ivry, S. W. Keele, H. C. Diener, Exp. Brain Res. 73, 167 (1988). 2. R. B. Ivry, H. S. Gopal, in Attention and Performance XIV, D. E. Meyer, S. Kornblum, Eds. (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1993), pp. 771– 802. 3. D. Timmann, S. Watts, J. Hore, Exp. Brain Res. 130, 441 (2000). 4. R. B. Ivry, S. W. Keele, J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1, 136 (1989). 5. J. A. Mangels, R. B. Ivry, N. Shimizu, Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 7, 15 (1998). 6. H. Ackermann, S. Graeber, I. Hertrich, I. Daum, Brain Lang. 60, 323 (1997). 7. I. Daum, M. M. Shugens, H. Ackerman, W. Lutzenberger, Behav. Neurosci. 107, 748 (1993). 8. H. Topka, J. Valls-Sole, S. G. Massaquoi, M. Hallett, Brain 116, 961 (1993). 9. D. S. Woodruff-Pak, M. Papka, R. B. Ivry, Neuropsychology 10, 443 (1996). 10. M. Jueptner et al., Neurology 45, 1540 (1995). 11. R. Kawashima et al., J. Neurophysiol. 83, 1079 (2000). 12. H. Ackermann, A. Riecker, K. Mathiak, M. Erb, W. Grodd, D. Wilddruber, Neuroreport 12, 4087 (2001). 13. R. B. Ivry, Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 41, 556 (1997). 14. J. Jackson, J. A. Michon, in Tutorials in Motor Neuroscience, J. Requin, G. Stelmach, Eds. (Kluwer, Norwell, MA, 1991), NATO Advanced Study Institute series, vol. 62, pp. 169 –198. 15. R. B. Ivry, Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 682, 214 (1993). 16. F. Mussa-Ivaldi, in Biomechanics and Neural Control of Posture and Movement, J. M. Winters, P. E. Crago, Eds. (Springer-Verlag, New York, 2000), pp. 325–333. 17. We focus on measures of variable error, operationalized as the standard deviation of the cycle durations. This measure has been commonly used in neuropsychological studies [e.g. (1)] to assess the contribution of different neural structures to timing based on the assumption that lesions add noise to the system. 18. S. D. Robertson et al., J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 25, 1316 (1999). 19. H. N. Zelaznik, R. M. Spencer, J. Doffin, J. Mot. Behav. 32, 193 (2000). 20. H. N. Zelaznik, R. M. Spencer, R. B. Ivry, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 28, 575 (2002). 21. By “explicit,” we mean that the action representation includes the specification of a temporal goal, as opposed to being a conscious representation as this term implies in the memory literature. 22. Materials and methods are available as supporting material on Science Online. 23. P. Killeen, N. Weiss, Psychol. Rev. 94, 455 (1987). 24. Variability due to linear drift in cycle duration was removed by detrending (34). A regression line was fit to the time series for each trial, and the residuals

25.

26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31.

were pooled across trials for each participant in each condition. The standard deviation of these residuals was divided by the mean cycle duration to obtain the coefficient of variation scores. One concern with our within-subject comparison is that the difference between hands may be related to other factors, such as handedness. However, the lesions affected the dominant hand for five of the six patients; thus, any effect of handedness would work against our hypothesis. G. Holmes, Brain 62, 1 (1939). A. J. Bastian, T. A. Martin, J. G. Keating, W. T. Thach, J. Neurophysiol. 76, 492 (1996). J. Hore, B. Wild, H. C. Diencer, J. Neurophysiol. 65, 563 (1991). S. W. Kennerley, J. Diedrichsen, E. Hazeltine, A. Semjen, R. B. Ivry, Nature Neurosci. 5, 376 (2002). M. T. Turvey, in Perceiving, Acting, and Knowing: Toward an Ecological Psychology, R. Shaw, J. Bransford, Eds. (Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1977), pp. 211–265. N. Hogan, T. Flash, Trends Neurosci. 10, 170 (1987).

32. R. J. van Beers, P. Baraduc, D. M. Wolpert, Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. 357, 1137 (2002). 33. E. Burdet, R. Osu, D. W. Franklin, T. E. Milner, M. Kawato, Nature 414, 446 (2001). 34. D. Vorberg, A. Wing, in Handbook of Perception and Action, Vol. 2: Motor Skills, H. Heuer, S. W. Keele, Eds. (Academic Press, San Diego, CA, 1996), pp. 181–262. 35. Supported by NSF dissertation improvement grant no. 0121930 (R.M.C.S.) and by NIH grant nos. NS30256, NS40813, and NS17778. We thank S. Keele and S. Grafton for comments on this manuscript. Supporting Online Material www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/300/5624/1437/ DC1 Materials and Methods Fig. S1 Tables S1 to S4 19 February 2003; accepted 30 April 2003

Characterization of Mammalian Selenoproteomes Gregory V. Kryukov,1 Sergi Castellano,2 Sergey V. Novoselov,1 Alexey V. Lobanov,1 Omid Zehtab,1 Roderic Guigo ´,2 1 Vadim N. Gladyshev * In the genetic code, UGA serves as a stop signal and a selenocysteine codon, but no computational methods for identifying its coding function are available. Consequently, most selenoprotein genes are misannotated. We identified selenoprotein genes in sequenced mammalian genomes by methods that rely on identification of selenocysteine insertion RNA structures, the coding potential of UGA codons, and the presence of cysteine-containing homologs. The human selenoproteome consists of 25 selenoproteins. In the universal genetic code, 61 codons encode 20 amino acids, and 3 codons are terminators. However, the UGA codon has a dual function in that it signals both the termination of protein synthesis and incorporation of the amino acid selenocysteine (Sec) (1–3). Available computational tools lack the ability to correctly assign UGA function. Consequently, there are numerous examples of misinterpretations of UGA codons as both Sec codons (4) and terminators (5, 6), including annotations of the human genome (7, 8), where no selenoproteins have been correctly predicted. With 18 human selenoprotein genes previously discovered (3), the estimates of the actual number of such genes vary greatly (9). All previously characterized selenoproteins except selenoprotein P (10) contain single Sec residues that are located in enzyme-active sites and are essential for their activity. Thus, misidentification of UGA 1 Department of Biochemistry, University of Nebraska, Lincoln, NE 68588–0664, USA. 2Grup de Recerca en Informa `tica Biome `dica, Institut Municipal d’Investigacio ´ Me `dica, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Centre de Regulacio ´ Geno `mica, Doctor Aiguader 80, 08003 Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain.

*To whom correspondence should be addressed. Email: [email protected]

codons leads to a loss of crucial biological and functional information. Sec is cotranslationally incorporated into nascent polypeptides in response to UGA codons when a specific stem-loop structure, designated the Sec insertion sequence (SECIS) element, is present in the 3⬘ untranslated regions (UTRs) in eukaryotes and in archaea, or immediately downstream of UGA in bacteria (1, 11–13). Trans-acting factors, including Sec tRNA, Sec-specific elongation factor, selenophosphate synthetase (SPS), Sec synthase, and a SECIS-binding protein, are also required for Sec biosynthesis and insertion (1, 3, 13–15). Most known selenoprotein genes have homologs, in which Sec is replaced with cysteine (Cys). However, these proteins are poor catalysts as compared with selenoproteins (3). We hypothesized that the UGA dualfunction problem could be solved by identifying selenoprotein genes in sequenced genomes and assigning terminator functions to the remaining in-frame UGAs. The requirement of SECIS elements for Sec insertion and the presence of Cys-containing homologs of selenoproteins suggested two independent bioinformatics methods for selenoprotein identification. In addition, we used an ob-

www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 300 30 MAY 2003

1439

Supporting Online Material Materials and Methods: The experimental protocol was approved by Institutional Review Boards from the University of California, the Veteran’s Administration Medical Center, Martinez, and Purdue University. Informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to testing. Seven individuals with focal, unilateral cerebellar lesions (see Figure S1), eight individuals with bilateral cerebellar degeneration, and ten neurologically healthy agematched adults participated in the experiments. The patients were recruited from an outpatient clinic or ataxia support group. Focal-lesion patients were only included when MRI and medical records indicated a lesion restricted to the cerebellum due to a single neurological incident (stroke or tumor). Degeneration patients were included only if there was no indication of extracerebellar pathology based on the analysis of brain scans, genetic subtyping, or clinical examination. Trajectory information was obtained with the miniBird magnetic tracking system (Ascension Technology, Burlington, VT). A 12 x 12 x 18 mm receiver was taped to the index finger and sampled at 144 Hz. Automated algorithms as described in the text were used to identify cycle onsets and, when required, cycle offsets. In all of the experiments, seven trials were completed for a given condition, with the first two trials serving as practice. Participants performed each task twice, once with the right arm and once with the left arm. The order of conditions was randomized across participants. Experiment 1: A synchronization-continuation task was used. The participants were presented with ten pacing tones, with a 400 ms inter-stimulus interval, followed by

a 16-s unpaced continuation phase. For the tapping task, participants were instructed tap the table with every other beat of the metronome using only the index finger. For the continuous circle drawing task, participants were instructed to complete one circle for every other beat of the metronome. For the intermittent drawing task, participants were instructed to trace a circle during one 400-ms interval, and pause at the top of the circle for the subsequent 400-ms interval. Both circling tasks involved movement about the shoulder and elbow. A template was provided to indicate the approximate target circle size (6-cm. diameter). Experiment 2: For the synchronization portion of the discrete tasks, three tones separated by 550 ms were presented. Participants attempted to initiate a movement coincident with the second tone and complete the circle or tapping interval coincident with the third tone. The inter-trial interval between each series of tones was randomly chosen between 1.5-2.5 s. For the continuation phase of the discrete tasks, participants were instructed to produce one tap interval or draw one circle after a randomly presented tone (1.8 - 2.8 s after previous response). A trial consisted of 10 synchronized and 20 continuation movements. Experiment 3: A synchronization-continuation task was used with a 500 ms intertone interval for the metronome in the continuous fast condition and 1000 ms inter-tone interval for the other three conditions.

Figure S1. Schematic reconstruction of the unilateral cerebellar lesions (in dark grey; light grey area represents secondary atrophy after resection). Seven horizontal sections, arranged down the column from superior to inferior, are shown for each patient. Lesions for Patients R02 and V05 include the dentate nucleus.

Table S1. Participant information. Exp: experiment(s) participated in. Edu: years of education. Onset: number of years between neurological insult and testing. Path: pathology of insult (CVA = cardiovascular accident; TUM = tumor; SCA = spinocerebellar ataxia genetic subtype (ref); ATRO = atrophy; the letter in parentheses represents the side of the lesion for the unilateral cerebellar patient group). The last three columns indicate clinical ratings on three representative tests of cerebellar dysfunction: Dys (Dysmetria, pointing accuracy), Ataxia (wrist pronation/supination), and Gait. Ratings are on 0-3 scale with 0 used to indicate normal performance and 3 used for severe impairment. Dysmetria and ataxia ratings for unilateral patients are for the ipsilesional limb. Deficits were generally symmetric in the bilateral patients. Subject Exp Gender Unilateral Cerebellars C01 1,2,3 M R02 1,2,3 M D03 1,2,3 M B04 1,2,3 M V05 1 M M06 1,2 M N07 2,3 M mean: Bilateral Cerebellars W11 2,3 F F12 2,3 F W13 2,3 M H14 2,3 M E15 2 M F16 2 M F17 2 M O18 3 M mean: Controls mean: 4F/6M

Age

Hand

Edu

Onset

Path

Dys

Ataxia

Gait

65 76 57 52 55 79 46 61.4

R L R R R R R

10 16 16 11 12 3 16 12.0

9 8 10 4 3 12 5

CVA(R) CVA (R) TUM(L) CVA (L) TUM(L) CVA(L) TUM(R)

3 0 1 2 3 0 3

3 1 1 2 3 0 3

0 1 0 2 0 0 1

41 44 72 63 87 63 46 50 58.2

R R R R R R R R

16 13 16 21 16 16 16 15 16.2

15 20 40 10 11 10 7 8

SCA6 SCA3 ATRO ATRO SCA6 SCA6 SCA3 SCA3

3 1 2 1 1 1 1 2

2 1 2 1 1 2 2 2

4 4 4 2 2 1 1 2

61.7

9R/1L

15.1

Table S2. Mean cycle duration for each task in Experiment 1. Standard errors, indicating between-subject variability in terms of movement rate, are presented in parentheses. Unimpaired Limb

Impaired Limb

844.7 (13.5)

775.3 (82.2)

Total, goal = 800 ms

1112.5 (86.8)

1351.1 (150.8)

Move, goal = 400 ms

801.1 (62.5)

962.0 (112.2)

Pause, goal = 400 ms

313.5 (27.7)

393.2 (41.7)

938.2 (139.8)

1032.8 (106.3)

Table Tapping Total, goal = 800 ms Intermittent Circle Drawing

Continuous Circle Drawing Total, goal = 800 ms

Table S3. Mean cycle duration for each task in Experiment 2. Values for the bilateral cerebellar and control groups are averages across dominant and non-dominant limbs since this variable did not affect performance. Unilateral Cerebellar

Bilateral Cerebellar

Matched Controls

Unimpaired

Impaired

Limb

Limb

524.7.2

573.9

459.2

494.2

(24.9)

(32.0)

(26.0)

(22.1)

914.8

943.9

1122.4

889.6

(73.7)

(57.4)

(72.1)

(64.4)

746.1

809.1

888.3

558.2

(110.8)

(79.3)

(88.2)

(22.3)

Discrete Tapping Goal = 550 ms

Discrete CircleDrawing Goal = 550 ms

Continuous CircleDrawing Goal = 550 ms

Table S4. Mean cycle duration for each task in Experiment 1. Values for the bilateral cerebellar and control groups are averages across dominant and non-dominant limbs since this variable did not affect performance. Unilateral Cerebellar

Bilateral Cerebellar

Matched Controls

Unimpaired

Impaired

Limb

Limb

960.2

976.5

882.3

972.0

(26.4)

(16.7)

(41.0)

(32.0)

1008.8

1054.2

974.0

954.6

(6.5)

(32.8)

(61.8)

(44.6)

998.6

978.3

992.8

1002.0

(47.1)

(36.2)

(52.1)

(45.2)

511.7

524.1

533.4

493.0

(15.5)

(18.0)

(47.7)

(12.2)

Table Tapping Goal = 1000 ms

Intermittent Tapping Goal = 1000 ms

Continuous Tapping Slow Goal = 1000 ms

Continuous Tapping Fast Goal = 500 ms